雅思閱讀素材積累:Now you know

字號:

夢想在前方,努力在路上。對于考生來說,拿到證書就是我們向往的遠(yuǎn)方。以下是“雅思閱讀素材積累:Now you know”,歡迎閱讀參考!更多相關(guān)訊息請關(guān)注!
    
    雅思閱讀:Now you know
    When should you teach children, and when should you let them explore?
    IT IS one of the oldest debates in education. Should teachers tell pupils
    the way things are or encourage them to find out for themselves? Telling
    children "truths" about the world helps them learn those facts more quickly. Yet
    the efficient learning of specific facts may lead to the assumption that when
    the adult has finished teaching, there is nothing further to learn—because if
    there were, the adult would have said so. A study just published in Cognition by
    Elizabeth Bonawitz of the University of California, Berkeley, and Patrick Shafto
    of the University of Louisville, in Kentucky, suggests that is true.
    Dr Bonawitz and Dr Shafto arranged for 85 four- and five-year-olds to be
    presented, during a visit to a museum, with a novel toy that looked like a
    tangle of coloured pipes and was capable of doing many different things. They
    wanted to know whether the way the children played with the toy depended on how
    they were instructed by the adult who gave it to them.
    One group of children had a strictly pedagogical introduction. The
    experimenter said "Look at my toy! This is my toy. I'm going to show you how my
    toy works." She then pulled a yellow tube out of a purple tube, creating a
    squeaking sound. Following this, she said, "Wow, see that? This is how my toy
    works!" and then demonstrated the effect again.
    With a second group of children, the experimenter acted differently. She
    interrupted herself after demonstrating the squeak by saying she had to go and
    write something down, thus suggesting that she might not have finished the
    demonstration. With a third group, she activated the squeak as if by accident.
    To a fourth, the toy was simply presented with the comment, "Wow, see this toy?
    Look at this!"
    After these varied introductions, the children were left with the toy and
    allowed to play. They might discover that, as well as the squeaker, the toy had
    a button inside one tube which activated a light, a keypad that played musical
    notes, and an inverting mirror inside one of the tubes. All the children were
    told to let the experimenter know when they had finished playing and were asked
    by the instructor if they were done if they stopped playing for more than five
    consecutive seconds. The entire interaction was recorded on video.
    Footage of each child playing was passed to a research assistant who was
    ignorant of the purpose of the study. The assistant was asked to record the
    total playing time, the number of different actions the child performed, the
    time spent playing with the squeak, and the number of other functions the child
    discovered.
    The upshot was that children in the first group spent less time playing
    (119 seconds) than those in the second (180 seconds), the third (133 seconds) or
    the fourth (206 seconds). Those in the first group also tried out four different
    actions, on average. The others tried 5.3, 5.9 and 6.2, respectively. A similar
    pattern (0.7, 1.3, 1.2 and 1.2) pertained to the number of functions other than
    the squeak that the children found.
    The researchers' conclusion was that, in the context of strange toys of
    unknown function, prior explanation does, indeed, inhibit exploration and
    discovery. Generalising from that would be ambitious. But it suggests that
    further research might be quite a good idea